|
The results of the task analyses and workload measurements have primarily provided objective confirmation for a number of intuitive beliefs about anesthesia practice. The real importance of these studies is that a coherent methodology has been developed that may be useful for studying a variety of interesting questions. The question of the impact of electronic automated record-keeping systems is addressed earlier. Another question concerns trans-esophageal echocardiography (TEE). This newer monitoring modality has become commonplace during cardiac anesthesia and anesthesia for other patients with cardiovascular disease who undergo complex operations. It is widely recognized that evaluating TEE images and manipulating the TEE probe require substantial visual and mental attention.
The UCSD/VA-Stanford group has published data suggesting that vigilance (as measured by response latency to illumination of a red light) was substantially lower when the anesthesiologist was involved with manipulating, adjusting, or examining the TEE compared with other patient care tasks ( Fig. 83-9 ).[110] This may be due, in part, to the layout of the workspace. The TEE machine is large and is often placed near the left side of the head of the OR table, whereas the anesthesia machine and attendant monitors, by convention, are typically placed near the right side of the head of the table. Remote displays slaved to the primary monitor are also more likely to be on the
Figure 83-9
Vigilance test data during anesthetic tasks with and
without automated record-keeping. The range of data is shown for each of four taskcategories
in electronic automated record-keeping (EARK) and manual record-keeping (MAN). Each
box contains 50% of the data for that subgroup (upper limit of box is the 75th percentile;
lower limit is the 25th percentile), whereas the maximum and minimum are shown by
the upper and lower horizontal bars. The response latency during record-keeping
was not significantly different between EARK and MAN. In both record-keeping groups,
subjects had significantly slower responses when observing or adjusting the transesophageal
echocardiogram (TEE), compared with recording, observing monitors, or adjusting intravenous
lines (IVs). Subjects in both groups had faster response latency when observing
the monitoring array, which contained a red light, compared with the other three
tasks. *, P < .05; †, P
< .05. (From Weinger MB, Herdon OW, Gaba DM: The effect of electronic
record keeping and transesophageal echocardiography on task distribution, workload,
and vigilance during cardiac anesthesia. Anesthesiology 87:144–145, 1997.)
Furthermore, it is important to differentiate the various uses of TEE. When it is used to answer specific clinical questions triggered by events or milestones during the procedure, the value of the information may be worth an investment of effort even though there is a reduction in overall vigilance. When it is used as a continuous monitor for myocardial ischemia, or when conducting a detailed routine examination, the reduction of vigilance must be considered when evaluating the potential benefits of the technology. Some practitioners describe special ways to handle the attention requirements of TEE, including tightening alarm thresholds (to provide audio warnings of changed values) and assigning specific patient monitoring functions to another individual while conducting the initial TEE placement and examination.
There remain many other interesting questions concerning the performance of anesthetists that can be addressed with the techniques of task analysis and workload and vigilance assessment, including the following:
Unlike in military aviation, there are few formal selection criteria for entry into anesthesia practice. Instead, the criteria are decided on by each department's trainee selection committee. Nearly every student who chooses anesthesia as a specialty will be accepted by some training institution. Moreover, few residents are terminated from their anesthesia training, and then usually only because of drug abuse or too persistent or egregious lack of normal or technical skill.
Incident | Anesthesia Residents (%) | Anesthesia Attendings (%) | Anesthesiologists in Practice (%) |
---|---|---|---|
Diagnosis of esophageal intubation | 80 | 100 | 100 |
Treatment of myocardial Ischemia | 20 | 40 | 20 |
Diagnosis of anaphylaxis | 20 | 60 | 40 |
Treatment of cardiac arrest | 40 | 30 | 20 |
From Schwid HA, O'Donnell D: Anesthesiologists' management of simulated critical incidents. Anesthesiology 76:495–501, 1992. |
There has been recent work on defining the underlying ability characteristics of the successful anesthetist. Greaves and Grant[31] presented an inventory of 16 characteristics of "good" anesthetic practice—knowledge, skill, perception, confidence, prudence, vigilance, fluency, decisiveness, anticipation, organization, flexibility, responsiveness, good manner, assertiveness, good management, and good communication. This inventory can serve as a basis for discussion of anesthetists' education and training. The authors of the inventory recommended its use for a formalized consultant feedback to their trainees, although they pointed out the unproven validity and reliability.
An interdisciplinary group in Germany conducted an evaluation of another list of critical abilities for anesthetists published in German.[243]
|